Cause & Effect?

Memory “illusions” may result from the basic human need to make sense out of events. A series of experiments has provided the first scientific evidence that when people see effects (a student toppling onto the floor) without also seeing its cause (a student leaning back in a chair), they automatically “fill in the blank” with that probable cause — even if they haven’t actually seen it with their own two eyes. The result: a memory that seems real, but isn’t. The inference may be correct, but it’s not based on actual perception, suggesting that memory helps us to make sense of the world, perhaps at the expense of a complete reliability. The findings are detailed in the July issue of the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, published by the American Psychological Association.

Psychologists Sharon L. Hannigan, Ph.D., of Bard College and Mark Tippens Reinitz, Ph.D., of the University of Puget Sound conducted three experiments (with 144 subjects in the first two and 48 in the third) that demonstrated “causal inference errors,” which are memory errors that result when people make inferences about the underlying causes of events. Hannigan and Reinitz showed subjects pictures depicting some kind of “effect,” such as oranges sprawled on a supermarket floor. They later showed pictures of the most probable cause of that effect, such as someone reaching for an orange from the bottom of a stack, and asked subjects whether they had seen that picture before. A statistically significant number said they did.

So, subjects filled in the gaps of missing scenes by saying they saw pictures they never actually saw — pictures that might have been expected to have been there in the first place. They inferred correctly, but their underlying memories were illusions. Thus, normal memory can be “tricked” in the way that optical illusions “trick” (or manipulate) normal visual perception. This finding confirms causal-inference error as a new category of inference-based memory error. “It is surprising that just a few minutes after seeing the effect scene, people would reliably claim to have seen the cause scene,” Reinitz comments. “After all, we tend to believe that we can accurately remember what we saw just a few minutes ago.”

Because memory for pictures tends to be very accurate and robust, the experiments used pictorial stimuli, not the more typically studied text stimuli, as a more rigorous test of the error. What’s more, “we put a lot of confidence in things that we have seen with our own eyes,” says Reinitz, “so applications to real-world situations are probably more varied and interesting than would be the case if we used text.”

Hannigan and Reinitz also found that errors increased with longer retention intervals. This finding has special importance for the courtroom, where cases go to trial many months after events occur — making eyewitnesses even more prone to inference-based errors than they already are. Causal inferences could also play an important role in relationships, in that people can misremember the causes of others’ behavior due to inference-based memory errors.

The researchers emphasize that their findings illuminate normal memory processes that are generally useful and adaptive. “More often than not,” Reinitz reiterates, “causal inferences are likely to be correct.”

Hannigan and Reinitz also found that these causal-inference errors were common in a backward, not forward direction — that is, exposure to “effect” slides caused illusory memories of seeing “cause” slides, but exposure to “cause” slides did not cause illusory memories of seeing “effect” slides. In their article, the researchers discuss possible reasons why people didn’t “fill in the blanks” when effect was missing, and call for additional research into this potentially significant phenomenon. Reinitz speculates that it may be due to a stronger primal need to ask, “Why?” than to ask “What would happen if….?”

Finally, the experimental results suggest that the underlying memory mechanisms that give rise to causal-inference errors are fundamentally different from those that give rise to schema-based memory errors, further evidence that “memory” is actually a collection of different, independent processes.

Comments are closed.

Intel Xeon E3-1281 V3 3.70GHz 4-core 8-thread 8MB 82W LGA1150 CPU processor picture

Intel Xeon E3-1281 V3 3.70GHz 4-core 8-thread 8MB 82W LGA1150 CPU processor

$49.90



Super-Micro P4DC6+ Motherboard with 2 Xeon i processors with SCSI RAID picture

Super-Micro P4DC6+ Motherboard with 2 Xeon i processors with SCSI RAID

$200.00



HP 1589 FMB-1101 Server Board w/ Intel Xeon E5-1603 @2.80GHz  w/ Ram & CPU Fan picture

HP 1589 FMB-1101 Server Board w/ Intel Xeon E5-1603 @2.80GHz w/ Ram & CPU Fan

$200.00



Windows / Linux VPS (Virtual Dedicated Server) 48GB RAM + 1500GB HDD + 3 months picture

Windows / Linux VPS (Virtual Dedicated Server) 48GB RAM + 1500GB HDD + 3 months

$329.99



Xeon Platinum CPU Clam Shell Case fit Intel Socket LGA4677 - Lot of 12 25 60 100 picture

Xeon Platinum CPU Clam Shell Case fit Intel Socket LGA4677 - Lot of 12 25 60 100

$16.90



Processor Tray LGA2011 for Intel Xeon E5 E7 E3 CPU - Lot of 2 6 12 30 & 50 - New picture

Processor Tray LGA2011 for Intel Xeon E5 E7 E3 CPU - Lot of 2 6 12 30 & 50 - New

$15.85



Xeon Socket LGA2066 Processor Tray for W2100 & W2200 CPU's Lot of 2 6 12 30 & 50 picture

Xeon Socket LGA2066 Processor Tray for W2100 & W2200 CPU's Lot of 2 6 12 30 & 50

$16.85



Xeon E5 E7 Processor Packaging for Tray LGA2011  52.5 x 45 mm - Lot of 2 6 12 30 picture

Xeon E5 E7 Processor Packaging for Tray LGA2011 52.5 x 45 mm - Lot of 2 6 12 30

$15.85



POSEIDON XEON D-1500 PRODRIVE TECH BOX picture

POSEIDON XEON D-1500 PRODRIVE TECH BOX

$500.00



Intel Xeon E7-4850V4 2.10GHz 16 core 32 threads 115W 40MB CPU processor picture

Intel Xeon E7-4850V4 2.10GHz 16 core 32 threads 115W 40MB CPU processor

$358.00



Powered by WordPress. Designed by WooThemes